X-Message-Number: 4986 Date: 14 Oct 95 16:53:36 EDT From: Mike Darwin <> Subject: SCI.CRYONICS:Response to Brad I thought Brad Templeton's questions about cold ischemia and synapse preservation were very good ones; I did not respond before because I wanted to see what Brian would say. I will try to address Brad's concerns: First, we have done experiments with 24 hours of flush-store on ice using dogs with NO warm ischemia and ideal treatment wherein they were put on bypass while alive, colled to 15 C and washed out and further cooled to 4C and then packed in ice (1-2 C) using both our own solution and Viaspan. This was followed by cold perfusion fixation, and TEM microscopy. Both solutions gave good preservation of synapses. However, to our surprise (and mixed pleasure/displeasure) the brains using our solution looked much better than the Viaspan brains. The principal differences were massive swelling of the axons (particularly the myleinated ones) and swelling of the mitochondria with lost of cristae in many neurons and glial cells in the Viaspan group. Our solution was indistinguishable from control (vitally perfused-fixed dogs) at 24 hours. Brad goes on to say: >I don't see how you could detect high-level memory loss easily in dogs. >What level of testing was done? Brian responds to this question by pointing out that dogs recovered from TBW still know their names and exhibit normal kennel behavior. This is nice to know, but it does not address Brad'd question, which is a VERY good one. Before I address it I will jump to a seemingly unrelated topic: In the last issue of Alcor's magazine CRYONICS there is an article by Thomas Donaldson on conservation of memory after ischemic insult. I had high hopes for this article but was very disappointed upon reading it. One thing which I have seen repeatedly cited by BOTH Donaldson and Merkle (AND for the same reasons!!!!!) is the story of Hossman's one hour ischemic cat which survived and recovered (walked around). This story is relevant because of it denouement and BECAUSE it bears directly on Brad's question. What about this marvelous cat?? What should we make of it? 1) Not much since only ONE cat survived and no one has come even close to duplicating this work. 2) Does anyone know what happened to this SPECIFIC margic cat? I do. It is an interesting story. Scientists are heartless souls and German scientists probably even more heartless (note I said heartless not malicious). The cat in question was allowed to wander around Hossman's lab for some months where it rubbed against people and generally behave like a cat. Which is to say it slept a lot, ate when it wanted and was the typical snotty cat like the one sitting on my lap (cats do, after all, look down on people). Finally they decided the cat's time had come and they anesthetized it and perfused it with fixative. They then looked at various brain areas. There were heavy neuron losses across the board. There were, and I quoting Hossman here, NO visible hippocampal neurons; just open space and scarring (fibrous tissue). There is a gruesome but relevant experiment you can do with mice. Mice have very thin skulls and you can see the brain through them. If you reflect the skin over a mouse's brain and repeatedly over a period of weeks progressively freeze the cerebral cortex through the bone you will eventually end up with a mouse with no upper cortical layers and just a series of sort of grayish empty pits visible under the bone. Such mice do quite well; they eat, run around and have sex. However, you cannot TEACH them anything. They are just little reptilian brains. As I sit here I am watching two large red-eared slider turtles. I just fed them and they are eating their Purina trout chow; they are incredibly stupid animals (but I love 'em anyway) and have barely learned, after all this time (a year) that the food is NOT where the motion is; they run up on the rocks where they see me, and then it takes them a long while to realize I have put the food in at the water end of the tank (this not cruelty or devilment; sliders cannot eat food except in the water; they would starve if I put it on the rocks). Brad is very perceptive: assessing neurological injury and in particular (and of most relevance to US) cognitive injury in dogs or other animals is not easy. I am moving beyond Peter Safar's overall performance catergories to much more sophisticated measures of canine intelligence and cognition. There is an excellent, indeed truly wonderful book that was published recently which describes objective ways to evaluate cognitive function in dogs. Further, we have developed sophisticated techniques of our own to do functional neurologicalevaluation. This is a VERY important and tricky business. BPI currently has a client who is dying. S/he is quite a brilliant person and has a mind that operates scattershot like a machine gun; jumping from subject to subject with lightning speed. I have called in as consults several of the best physicians in the United States to deal with various aspects of managing this patient's care. One consult flew in from the East Coast. One of the things I noticed about this patient was his/her noncompliance and seeming inability to follow directions. It took me an actual MEETING with the person before I realized what was wrong: s/he had profound cognitive impairment; almost no short-term memory. While the person was witty, appropriate, fully oriented to person, place and time, and able to give complex instructions to get me to an eatery for lunch, s/he was unable to remember a number given when we paused during our wide ranging conversation in the following manner: "Mr/Ms X, I want to interrupt our conversation here. This is very important. I am going to ask you to remember a number in order to see how good your memory for new things is. The number is 453. Can you remember that?" The MD got an irritated "of course!" as a response. Five minutes later he stopped the conversation and asked for the number; the person could only NOT give the MD the number, indeed s/he had some initial puzzlement as to what he was aking for! Because of the thinking style of this individual (very nonlinear and nontechnical) this devastating loss of short term memory was almost invisible. This person was not and is not demented and does not have Alzheimer's, but rather is suffering from a serious but fairly compartmentalized cognitive defect. It was of great clinical significance to identify this defect since it was preventing medical compliance, preventing intake of premeds, resulting in overdoses of other prescribed meds (didn't remember taking the pills, took MORE) and most importantly was resulting in severe malnutrition since this person was preparing his/her own meals! To belabor the point further, Wilder Penfield (the father of modern neurosurgery) had a patient with severe BILATERAL temporal lobe epilepsy which was uncontrolled by drugs. Penfield operated on monkeys ablating BOTH of the areas in the temporal lobes he intended to ablate in this patient. The monkeys did fine; they remembered everything they had been taught and appeared normal. Penfield then operated on his young human patient. Everything went swimingly well! The patient woke up, the seizures were gone, and he knew everybody. The problem was detected a few dys later when it became apparent that he had essentially NO short-term memory. If he met someone for the first time and they walked out of the room to return 10 minutes later he had no idea who they were. As of some years ago this unfortunate man was living in the same house he had since childhood with the same issues of Reader's Digest sitting around, the same furniture in the same place. If he goes outside and sees new cars and jet planes he becomes excited and confused. He is thus marooned in the mid-1950's. When Penfield went back and checked his monkeys, he found they too were marooned in the past. It just wasn't obvious. We have become VERY sensitive to this issue in dogs in our TBW and ischemia experiments. Dogs that LOOK just fine will sometimes have almost no neurons left in multiple areas of their brains. This work ain't simple! >Clearly the answer is simple, to examine perfused tissues in animals after >a long cold ischemia period matching the pattern of a typical suspension, >look at the detailed neural structure, see how much it has degraded and >speculate on whether it can be repaired, and how to improve it. Again Brad is right on the money. As I said, we have done work on nonshocked dogs after 24 hours cold ischemia. But what we need to do is shock/global ischemia models as well, which paralell the long agonal courses of patients with SYSTOLIC blood pressures of 50 mmHg or BELOW who lay there with pupils midposition and unresponsive to light for 1-2 days in this condition before dying! If you doubt me on this, or how common it is, I suggest you call up your local home-hospice or freestanding in-patient hospice and talk to a few of the nurses there. They'll set you straight in a hurry. >How much does such work cost? I presume it requires electron microscopy. Yes, currently we are doing EM. But this is hardly the whole picture. There is a new form of vital light microscopy which one of our investigators flew out to DuPont to see. Very impressive: EM resolution without artifact on living tissue. The hardware is exotic and out of our price range, but we are working on other ways to get access to this technology. Further, there are lovely probes that can be used to image and evaluate brain tissue function in slices and in whole brains such as voltage sensitive dyes and "functional connection mapping using intracelluar electrodes.... Also, freeze fracture (a kind of EM) could probably tell us a lot. But we have had real problems here getting images of CPA-loaded systems which are interpretable. Again, time and money have limited me in following this up. Unlike CI who rely on workers overseas, I consider the details of EM prep-work and execution to be critically important to getting good results. I fly up initially when the first batch is run, and I fly up when the tissue is scanned. I have tried "cheap" EM services. You get what you pay for. Excluding the animal, surgical, disposables, chemicals and other prep costs to simulate the typical human cryopreservation, EM is comparatively cheap. We work up 3 blocks from each of 12 brain areas on a full survey and get an average of 6-12 pictures per block. This is a LOT of EM!!!!!!! Cost for such a complete work-up are in the 4-6K range depending on how many different magnifications I want. I think this is a very good price, especially considering they have to deal with moi! The cost (marginal) to fully simulate a human cryopresevation (excluding prolonged shock) is about 3K to 5K depeding on personnel availability, and so on (some folks work for free; actually the noncryonicists are some of my best volunteers, not that there aren't as many or more committed cryonicists as volunteers). I have NOT done prolonged shock models due to the cost and the certain knowledge that I will have to run through some animals to get the model worked out. If anyone is willing to pay for such work, I'm willing to do it, indeed anxious to. Right now, it is not so much of a priority for me because I am focused on solving the big underlying problem which is at the root of WHY so many patients end up experiencing this kind of insult: improper or inadequate pre-arrest pharmacologic cerebral protection, development of appropriate reperfusion techniques after global and trickle-flow ischemic injury, and finally, reversible brain cryopreservation. There is only so much time (and money) I can spend on survey work (such as finding the answers to Brad's good questions and the many variants that spring fom them such as: well are synapses intact after 2 hours of global ischemia followed by washout? What is the effect of a prolonged, unsuccessful "code" on ultrastructure after TBW, etc.). I have to decide where I must profitably put my time. I have however, notified my customers that I am giving notice on all BPI cryopreservation contract services. This is being done because I wish to add some language relating to media and precryopreservation care, and because I do NOT intend to more human cases unless I can take small, clinically acceptable (by today's standards) brain biopsies from carefully selected areas of the nondominant hemisphere. I am tired of not having feedback and there is no substitue for REAL patients, for HUMANS. And there is no substitute for this feedback in achieving quality control. Brian alludes to this in his response; since he opened the door, I've walked through it. Finally I note that Donalson in his CRYONICS article notes that monkeys can recover from long periods (15+ minutes) of global brain ischemia. This is true and this WHY we don't use them. Dogs are MORE like people than monkeys; especially monkeys that swing from trees. It's a wild guess but I'd bet that brain protection and repair systems are upregulated in monkeys (this could be a useful area for study in and of itself!). Whatever the reasons, the outcome, the histology and the ultrastructure of monkey brains in resoponse to ischemia DO NOT map what happens in humans well at all. Dogs do: and this is why use them. Cats seem to be intermediate between the two. As to the notion that humans are being salvaged over comparable periods of time this is nonsense. Yes, you can find anecdotal reports, but there are no RCTs that I know of or even large-scale non RCT studies. Donaldson remarks that: "By now some neurologists have learned to use several drugs (nimodipine, naloxone, pentobarbital) to lessen these effects (sic) of brain ischemia. I would like to see the hard data here. I personally saw the early RCTs underway with Naloxone by Toth at Methodist hospital in 1979-80. It didn't work. Recent large studies on Nimodipine in the clinical setting (i.e., not in the lab) showed DECREASED survival and increased morbidity, probably due to the negative impact of the drug on blood pressure: if there's one thing Hossman and Safar have taught us, its the importance of a hypertensive bout following the ischemic interval (in part to open up capillaries full of hyperviscous blood and ATP depleted and thus rigid RBC and WBCs (water quickly gets extracted from the boood and absorbed by the swelling glial cells and neurons and this increases the colloid concentration of the capillary blood enormously making it thick and resistant to reflow). One reason that this hypertensive bout has not been applied clinicaly is that hearts STOPPED by heart-attack and subjected to ischemia while fibrillating (using about 300 times their resting engery requirements!) don't easily deliver mean arterial pressures (MAPS) of 150 mmhg!!!!! As to pentobarbital; it is no longer widely used. In our hands it has had NO protective effect given before or after ischemia of more than 10 minutes. Given before in shorter episodes it is modestly protective in very high doses. Given after, not protective in terms of outcome (neurological recovery) as far as we can see. It also depresses blood pressure which is definitely the kiss of death in and of itself. A long answer, and an answer to some questions not asked :). But I make no apologies. Mike Darwin Rate This Message: http://www.cryonet.org/cgi-bin/rate.cgi?msg=4986